In an abysmal article, Nautilus dismisses the importance of genes

March 26, 2024 • 11:30 am

This is one of the worst papers on genetics I’ve seen in the last 15 years, and although it’s from 2019, this same kind of palaver keeps coming around again and again, and in exactly the same form. And so when a reader sent me the link, I reacted instinctively. The laws of physics mandated that, like a starving leopard encountering an antelope, I must fall on it and rip it to pieces.  So here goes. (Yes, Carole Hooven is right: males tend to have the killer instinct more than do females!)

The piece is intended not for professionals but for laypeople, and appeared in Nautilus, a quarterly magazine on science and its relationship to and implications for society. Founded by a big grant from the John Templeton Foundation, it does publish solid science articles, but sometimes the Foundation’s purpose (to find evidence of God in science) shines through. This occurs through promoting bizarre science, like panpsychism, or touting dubious reconciliations between religion and science. This paper falls into a third class: doing down “modern” genetics to imply that there’s something terribly wrong with our modern paradigm. (Evolution is a related and favorite target.)

The author, Ken Richardson, seems to have derived most of his genetics from fringe figures like Denis Noble and James Shapiro, with the result that the casual, non-geneticist reader will buy what these people are selling: genes are of only minor significance in both development and evolution.

Richardson is listed in the article as “formerly Senior Lecturer in Human Development at the Open University (U.K.). He is the author of Genes, Brains and Human Potential: The Science and Ideology of Intelligence.”

Read it by clicking below, or find the article archived here.

I was torn between ignoring this paper—for the author deserves no attention—or taking it apart. I decided on a compromise: to show some of the statements it makes that are either flat wrong or deeply misguided. Richardson’s quotes are indented, and my take is flush left. Here’s how he starts:

The preferred dogma started to appear in different versions in the 1920s. It was aptly summarized by renowned physicist Erwin Schrödinger in a famous lecture in Dublin in 1943. He told his audience that chromosomes “contain, in some kind of code-script, the entire pattern of the individual’s future development and of its functioning in the mature state.”

Around that image of the code a whole world order of rank and privilege soon became reinforced. These genes, we were told, come in different “strengths,” different permutations forming ranks that determine the worth of different “races” and of different classes in a class-structured society. A whole intelligence testing movement was built around that preconception, with the tests constructed accordingly.

The image fostered the eugenics and Nazi movements of the 1930s, with tragic consequences. Governments followed a famous 1938 United Kingdom education commission in decreeing that, “The facts of genetic inequality are something that we cannot escape,” and that, “different children … require types of education varying in certain important respects.”

The “strengths” and “permutations of genes” was not widely viewed as the underpinnings of different races. Yes, racial hierarchies were constructed based on supposed genetic constitution, but not the image of the “code script”.  It was the claim that racial differences were inherited, regardless how inheritance worked—much less the unproved notion of “code script”—that buttressed the Nazis’ eugenics program.  But somehow Richardson manages to connect the Nazis with the genetic code at the very beginning of his paper. But this is a minor quibble compared to what follows.

Richardson then uses what he sees as the disappointing performance of the GWAS (Genome-wide Association Studies) method (used to locate, from population surveys, regions of the genome responsible for various traits, which helps narrow down the location of “candidate genes”):

Now, in low-cost, highly mechanized procedures, the search has become even easier. The DNA components—the letters in the words—that can vary from person to person are called single nucleotide polymorphisms, or SNPs. The genetic search for our human definition boiled down to looking for statistical associations between such variations and differences in IQ, education, disease, or whatever.

For years, disappointment followed: Only a few extremely weak associations between SNPs and observable human characteristics could be found. Then another stroke of imagination. Why not just add the strongest weak associations together until a statistically significant association with individual differences is obtained? It is such “polygenic scores,” combining hundreds or thousands of SNPs, varying from person to person, and correlating (albeit weakly) with trait scores such as IQ or educational scores, that form the grounds for the vaulting claims we now witness.

Today, 1930s-style policy implications are being drawn once again. Proposals include gene-testing at birth for educational intervention, embryo selection for desired traits, identifying which classes or “races” are fitter than others, and so on. And clever marketizing now sees millions of people scampering to learn their genetic horoscopes in DNA self-testing kits.

So the hype now pouring out of the mass media is popularizing what has been lurking in the science all along: a gene-god as an entity with almost supernatural powers. Today it’s the gene that, in the words of the Anglican hymn, “makes us high and lowly and orders our estate.”

Although GWAS studies are hard and require big samples, and give genomic regions rather than genes there have been some notable successes in both medical genetics and agriculture, as one would expect in the past five years (see this Twitter thread for some examples).  The implication throughout the paper is that the failure of GWAS to locate individual genes responsible for traits shows that the variation of genes themselves aren’t responsible for the variation in traits. There must be something else!

But that’s completely wrong. We already have a way to judge the influence of genetic variation on trait variation, and that is heritability analysis. Heritabilities (symbolized as h²) range between 0 and 1, and are a measure of the proportion of variation for a trait in a given population caused by the variation among the genes in that population (the rest is due to environmental variation, interactions between genes and environments, and other arcane factors). But the point is that heritabilities calculated from our earlier crude methods are nearly always higher than heritabilities estimated from GWAS analysis, simply because GWAS (but not h²) misses a lot of variable gene sites that have small effects, and isn’t good at detecting effects of rare alleles. But the more we use GWAS, the more variation we find, and, for well studied traits like height, heritabilities estimated from traditional methods are now converging with heritabilities estimated from GWAS.

And heritabilities of most traits, which are most extensively studied in humans, are often quite high. Have a look at this list, for example, which includes cognitive traits, behavioral traits, and physical traits. Most heritabilities range between 0.2 and 0.8, which means that for a typical trait, between 20% and 80% of the inter-individual variation in a population is due to variation of genes. When asked to guess the heritability of an unknown trait, I’d usually say, “well, probably about 50%”.  That seems, for example, to be close to the heritability of IQ in a population.

This shows that genes are highly important in explaining human variation, just as they are variation in animals and plants. This phenomenon was well known ages ago. If genes weren’t important in variation, selective breeding of dogs, plants, pigeons, and so on would be almost useless. Here’s a famous quote from Darwin’s in The Origin:

“Breeders habitually speak of an animal’s organization as something quite plastic, which they can model almost as they please.”

If genes weren’t important in variation, animals (and plants, which of course have been bred out the wazoo) wouldn’t be so plastic. Ergo genetic variation is important in explaining the variation of organisms.

Despite this, Richardson makes the following statement, which would astound most geneticists:

. . . . it is now well known that a group of genetically identical individuals, reared in identical environments—as in pure-bred laboratory animals—do not become identical adults. Rather, they develop to exhibit the full range of bodily and functional variations found in normal, genetically-variable, groups. In a report in Science in 2013, Julia Fruend and colleagues observed this effect in differences in developing brain structures.

Full range? Really? Yes, there is still variation among clonal individuals raised in identical environments, but not nearly as much as among genetically variable individuals raised in different environments! Clonal populations show a heritability of zero (they have no genetic variation among them), so there is less phenotypic variation among the individuals.  As for the Fruend paper, it shows plasticity of brain development, because of course learning is a form of adaptive plasticity that can change the brain. But that by no means says that genes aren’t an important source of variation.

I could go on and on about how Richardson claims that genes aren’t important, all the while showing that they are. Here’s a good example:

First, laboratory experiments have shown how living forms probably flourished as “molecular soups” long before genes existed. They self-organized, synthesized polymers (like RNA and DNA), adapted, and reproduced through interactions among hundreds of components. That means they followed “instructions” arising from relations between components, according to current conditions, with no overall controller: compositional information, as the geneticist Doron Lancet calls it.

In this perspective, the genes evolved later, as products of prior systems, not as the original designers and controllers of them. More likely as templates for components as and when needed: a kind of facility for “just in time” supply of parts needed on a recurring basis.

So what? There were primitive replicators first, which might as well be called genes, but the modern system of sophisticated gene action, often involving introns, splicing, transcription factors, and so on, is what we know about now, and what Richardson says about early organisms is irrelevant.  But wait! There’s more!

Then it was slowly appreciated that we inherit just such dynamical systems from our parents, not only our genes. Eggs and sperm contain a vast variety of factors: enzymes and other proteins; amino acids; vitamins, minerals; fats; RNAs (nucleic acids other than DNA); hundreds of cell signalling factors; and other products of the parents’ genes, other than genes themselves.

Where does Richardson think that those enzymes and proteins come from, which are often used to manufacture vitamins and amino acids? Where do the cell signalling factors come from? They all come from genes! The “dynamical systems” that he touts so highly come largely from genes, and without genes we would have no organisms and no evolution. Yes, environmental factors are important in controlling the timing and action of genes, but often those “environmental factors”, like signals in different organs that lead to differential development, are themselves derived from genes. And the sequestration and use of externally derived chemicals, like some amino acids and vitamins, are also controlled by genes.

I can barely go on, and if I continue this would last forever. Just one or two more pieces of stupidity:

Accordingly, even single cells change their metabolic pathways, and the way they use their genes to suit those patterns. That is, they “learn,” and create instructions on the hoof. Genes are used as templates for making vital resources, of course. But directions and outcomes of the system are not controlled by genes. Like colonies of ants or bees, there are deeper dynamical laws at work in the development of forms and variations.

Some have likened the process to an orchestra without a conductor. Physiologist Denis Noble has described it as Dancing to the Tune of Life (the title of his recent book). It is most stunningly displayed in early development. Within hours, the fertilized egg becomes a ball of identical cells—all with the same genome, of course. But the cells are already talking to each other with storms of chemical signals. Through the statistical patterns within the storms, instructions are, again, created de novo. The cells, all with the same genes, multiply into hundreds of starkly different types, moving in a glorious ballet to find just the right places at the right times. That could not have been specified in the fixed linear strings of DNA.

My answer is “yes it could have, and it is”. Those “chemical systems” that cause an organism to develop come from genes, which have changed over evolutionary time in a way that leads to adaptations, including proper development. By and large, genes control development, particularly early development.  Organisms with pretty much the same genes (members of the same species, for example) always turn out pretty much alike, with similar behaviors and appearances. Further, the more closely related species are, the more similar they tend to be. This reflects genetic similiarity, not some nebulous similarity in “dynamical systems,” whatever those are.

One more:

But it’s not so simple. Consider Mendel’s sweet peas. Some flowers were either purple or white, and patterns of inheritance seemed to reflect variation in a single “hereditary unit,” as mentioned above. It is not dependent on a single gene, however. The statistical relation obscures several streams of chemical synthesis of the dye (anthocyanin), controlled and regulated by the cell as a whole, including the products of many genes. A tiny alteration in one component (a “transcription factor”) disrupts this orchestration. In its absence the flower is white.

This is a good illustration of what Noble calls “passive causation.” A similar perspective applies to many “genetic diseases,” as well as what runs in families. But more evolved functions—and associated diseases—depend upon the vast regulatory networks mentioned above, and thousands of genes. Far from acting as single-minded executives, genes are typically flanked, on the DNA sequence, by a dozen or more “regulatory” sequences used by wider cell signals and their dynamics to control genetic transcription.

“Statistical relation”? What is described in peas is a direct causal relation: a mutation, acting through pathways, is responsible for changing flower color. If you flip a light switch, the light goes on. If you have the right mutation, the flower is white. What’s the big deal? Further, “transcription factors” are coded in the DNA; they are proteins that regulate the transcripotion of other genes: how those genes make messenger RNA.

And the ultimate dissing of genes:

We have reached peak gene, and passed it.

Finally, because GWAS studies aren’t yet developed to the point where they always can pick out important genes (remember, variation in most traits is due to variation in many genes, with the variants having small effects, and GWAS misses rare genes), Richardson says this:

The startling implication is that the gene as popularly conceived—a blueprint on a strand of DNA, determining development and its variations—does not really exist.

Well, as Dawkins has pointed out, genes are more like “recipes” than blueprints, but this isn’t what Richardson is saying here. What he is saying is that genes play at best only a small role in development.  He is both wrong and muddled.

It is this kind of popular science that I most despise, because it dissimulates, misleads, and even fibs about the state of modern science. By misleading the public about genetics, it affects not only their understanding of science, but, when shown up to be nonsense, as I and other have done, erodes public trust in science.

If you want to read this piece, be my guest, but if you know anything about genetics, keep a big glass of Pepto-Bismol at hand.

 

h/t: the always helpful Luana

33 thoughts on “In an abysmal article, Nautilus dismisses the importance of genes

  1. “We have reached peak gene, and passed it.” Makes me wish for the day we can say, “We have reached peak Creationist Histrionics, and moved on to better use of our time.”

  2. He was making me gag aplenty with the blather surrounding the origin of life. And then of course he cites Denis Noble.

  3. It is telling IMHO that Marx’s Hermetic conception of man making society making man – i.e. man as god – never gets attention for its own eugenics program. See Ersatz Religion in

    Science, Politics, and Gnosticism
    Eric Voegelin
    1968, 1997, Regenery Press, Chicago;Washington D.C.

    Or also

    Economic and Philosophic Manuscripts of 1844
    Karl Marx

    Fascism – which, as far as I can tell is what Nationalsozialismus was – is the dialectical negation – i.e. enemy cousin – of communism. So one wonders, how and why the easy enemy target bashing of Nazism is being used here.

    Doctrine of Fascism
    Benito Mussolini
    1932
    The version I read is here, maybe there’s a better one :
    http://www.worldfuturefund.org/wffmaster/Reading/Germany/mussolini.htm

    Hitler also wrote about how Marx’s ideas influenced him, but I don’t have the quote handy.

  4. You have a stronger stomach than I, to be able to read and critique this. I fear I would need not just Pepto but some IV Tagamet and a double dose of Esomeprazole. Thanks for your response to it.

  5. Although of course very different in both scholarship and emphasis, this reminded me of Lewontin’s book (with Rose and Kamin) Not In Our Genes.
    Has our host read/reacted to that one?

      1. The Nautilus article makes explicit a hostility toward the science of Genetics per se which has been fermenting on the pop-Left for many years. Alas, one could blame Dick Lewontin’s bugbear about “biological determinism”, at least in part, for fostering attitudes on display in the Richardson hogwash. In the 1970s (when I got to know Dick slightly) a little skepticism toward the DNA hype being broadcast by overenthusiastic molecular biologists was appropriate. But even then, DNAphobia of the Richardson variety was already getting indigestible.
        On the one hand, DNAphobia was typical of certain old-timey biologists (especially of the “theoretical biology” school) who feared for their status. On the other hand, it was seized on by pop-Leftists of the “science for the People” category. Baffling that someone as intelligent as Dick Lewontin didn’t see where DNAphobia was going.
        Where it went, as displayed in the Richardson screed, is still another example of Sturgeon’s Law.

  6. Have I misunderstood Jerry? You say

    Clonal populations show a heritability of zero …

    I thought that clones had a heritability of 1.

    1. Heritability is (approximately) the fraction of phenotypic variation that can be attributed to genetic variation. Clones have no genetic variation, therefore heritability is 0. In other words, any phenotypic variation you observe must be due to something other than genetic differences among individuals.

    2. What I meant is that if you have a clonal population of individuals, all with the same genotype, and you select on existing phenotypic variation to get, say, a bigger plant, you will make NO progress. That’s because all the selected variation is due to the environment. Another way of looking at heritability is R/S, where R is the response to selection and S is the selection pressure (mean diff. between selected and total individuals). No matter what S is, there will be no response, for the heritability is zero. You can’t make progress by selecting on a population with no genetic variation among individuals. Does that clear matters up?

      1. No Pepto-Bismol in the house so I settled for your incisive review.

        If keto-enol tautomerism (or one of the variations) causes a mis-pairing during DNA replication and introduces a mutation, I suppose the offspring are no longer strictly clonal even though their parents started out that way. I’m not nit-picking, I’m just making sure I understand how heritability could change from an initial value of zero in a clonal population, and then selection could start operating with preservation of the observed variation if not lethal.

  7. “The cells, all with the same genes, multiply into hundreds of starkly different types, moving in a glorious ballet to find just the right places at the right times. That could not have been specified in the fixed linear strings of DNA.”

    Personal incredulity (which I heard of first from Richard Dawkins, but which has a longer history) is not an argument. I, for one, have no problem thinking that all of those things are specified in the genes. And it’s not that I’m just more gullible. It’s because of the incontrovertible evidence.

    1. Personal incredulity is also the go-to informal logical fallacy of creationists (“well, I just don’t see how a bacterial flagellum could have evolved”).

      1. I particularly like how that debate tactic subtly flips the onus around so now the evolutionary biologist is almost forced to improvise an explanation for the evolution of the flagellum on the spot – which nobody can really do well – “proving” the creationist “right”.

        The equivalent of swatting the chess pieces off the board.

  8. In jest, a professor and friend once wrote to me that one can make whatever claims one wants about what authors of books, etc. said long ago, because nobody has read those books for 50 years. I would add that this goes double when the topic involves genetics, IQ, etc.

    As such, when I am confronted with the likes of this article by Richardson, I check the primary sources. The results are predictable.

    In the present case, I homed in on the quote “The facts of genetic inequality are something that we cannot escape”. Richardson, in a previous article [1], attributed this quote to The 1938 Spens Report [2].

    Try as I might, I could not find the quote in the background or full text (please, try and prove me wrong). The report doesn’t actually talk about genes. At all. Also, the only ‘races’ it speaks of are “the human race”, “the race”, and “civilised races”, and in all cases (races?), the discussion of these things is pretty anodyne.

    The other part of the quote “different children … require types of education varying in certain important respects.” can be found on page 125 of the report. The quote is “Different children from the age of 11, if justice is to be done to their varying capacities, require types of education varying in certain important respects.”

    If you continue to read the report, you see that it refers to tailoring education to meet the needs and talents of different children. Shocking stuff, I know.

    The summary on Wikipedia [3] seems quite good, although I have not read it in full. It summarizes all the dastardly things it recommended, including raising school leaving age to 16 and abolishing school fees.

    [1] https://www.bps.org.uk/psychologist/so-what-gene
    [2] https://education-uk.org/documents/spens/spens1938.html
    [3] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Spens_Report

  9. Memo to Ken Richardson:

    A political answer to a scientific question is always incorrect.

    Or, would you care to explain why you are not a zebra? Or why identical twins look the same?

  10. “The DNA components—the letters in the words—that can vary from person to person are called single nucleotide polymorphisms, or SNPs.”

    What? What about multiple nucleotide polymorphisms?

    Oh, yeah, and everything else you said.

  11. The quoted passages show some mystification – culminating in this final over-the-top mystification :

    “The startling implication is that the gene as popularly conceived—[…]—does not really exist.”

    See? The author is saying the popular conception of the gene mystifies the truth – come with us, we have the secret knowledge nobody else can tell you, dear reader.

    Only certain categories of writing play these games – with any subject.

  12. I wonder, Jerry, if you have seen today’s article in Aeon by Charudatta Navare.

    The gist of it is that several feminist scientists have debunked the false notion that the instructions for a cell are contained in the nucleus and that the nucleus does not actually control the cell. The only reason this notion came about in first place is because science is dominated by males who live in a top-down society.

    It’s a good one. I think you will enjoy it!

  13. Thanks, Jerry, very interesting and useful, and I completely agree with you. To widen this out to the big picture though, it seems to be just one front in a war against the modern synthesis in evolutionary theory, and this for me is rooted in fear. The fact that evolution happened means we are animals – and it seems to me that many people just can’t stomach the idea, which prompted the – sadly recently departed – primatologist, Frans de Waal to propose the term anthropo-denial for people’s failure to understand our proper place in nature (I’d be inclined to be less charitable and call it anthropocentric arrogance). And the motivation, and justification for this arrogance, can be summed up in one word: “human”. There’s of course, nothing wrong with using this word if it is applied as denoting the difference between this species of animal and, say, the chimpanzee kind of animal, but that is not how it is usually interpreted. For cultural, historical and religious reasons people in the West, and probably most other places, are conditioned in our upbringing to think of “human” to be in contradistinction to “animal”.
    Obviously, you won’t need any persuasion to accept what I say in this paragraph, but at the species level, every advanced animal is defined, in respect of its morphology, physiology and behaviour by its genes and the way they are expressed, and this must include the human animal. Which is not, of course, to say that the level below, that of the individual, other factors like environment, upbringing and development aren’t important, And, of course, if there weren’t genetic differences between individual animals natural selection would not work.
    If the general behavioural aspects of the modern human species are under the control of genes, and, given that we did not evolve to live in the world in which we are living now, and that we evolved under the auspices of the arbitrary, mindless and soulless mechanisms of evolutionary processes like natural selection, isn’t it time to have an audit of which behaviours are appropriate in our world today? For example, what seem to be human universal behaviours, like, patriotism and nationalism are strongly felt by many to be noble, just and proper traits to have, but if that’s true, were they noble, just and proper in Germany in the 1930s/40s?
    That’s one of the reasons why, I think, this war against science is so dangerous, it stops us seeing our species as it really is. And yes, of course, I know how radical the ideas I’m proposing are, but heliocentrism, big bang theory, continental drift (now renamed plate tectonics) and, of course, evolution itself were all radical ideas, and they were all subsequently confirmed – radicalism in an idea does not necessarily imply its falsity.
    There is an old trope that “the truth will out” and we have evidence and reason on our side, so let’s hope that this attack on science and other parallel nonsenses like panpsychism and postmodernism will in time be recognised to be the ridiculous fads they are.

  14. Anyone who thinks Mendel’s crossing experiments were done with sweet peas (Lathyrus odoratus) lacks credibility. (Perhaps he was thinking about Bateson and Punnett.)

  15. A dirty job, even reading, let alone critiquing this paper, whyevolutonistrue, (aka Jerry, but I don’t know you), but someone hadda…coming in late with my thanks.

    This blog — I’m frequently in agreement, take a break, seem to always return. Does it fuel my insomnia — or is the insomnia intrinsic, ready to chew on whatever disturbing information crosses my mind?

  16. At the end of one of Sabine’s recent videos, she hawks Nautilus Magazine with a full-throated endorsement ($$$), along with the inclusion of her own articles therein.

    The Templeton Foundation continues to usurp the reputations of real scientists (Jane Goodall; Martin Rees; even Sean Carroll, who somehow serves on their board of advisors, if I recall correctly) in order to usurp science itself (“Without Christianity, or something “beyond”, or some supernatural mind, there wouldn’t be any science!”, to paraphrase).

    Just disappointing.

Leave a Comment

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *