Still more critique of the PLOS article on women hunting in hunter-gatherer societies

July 10, 2023 • 9:45 am

A recent paper in PLOS ONE by Anderson et al. concluded that the frequency of hunter-gatherer societies in which women hunted was much higher than people thought (79%), which was taken to discard the “myth” that in such societies there was a sex-based division of labor, with men hunting and women foraging. Further, Anderson et al. concluded that in 37% of the societies women hunted large game or game of all sizes, so that the hunting was not just things like birds or rabbits. That was taken to explode another myth—that of women who didn’t hunt the big game.

Here’s what I wrote about the paper on June 30:

On June 30 I reported on a PLOS One paper whose data showed that, in existing hunter-gatherer societies, women participated in hunting a lot more than most people (well, at least I) thought.  (The original paper is here.) Here’s what I wrote, giving the authors’ conclusions (the authors’ words are doubly indented, my short bit singly indented).

The authors’ conclusions:

Here we investigated whether noted trends of non-gendered hunting labor known from the archaeological record continued into more recent, ethnographic periods. The descriptive sample described here is sufficient to warrant the conclusion that women in foraging societies across the world participate in hunting during more recent time periods, a finding that makes sense given women’s general morphology and physiology . The prevalence of data on women hunting directly opposes the common belief that women exclusively gather while men exclusively hunt, and further, that the implicit sexual division of labor of ‘hunter/gatherer’ is misapplied. Given that this bimodal paradigm has influenced the interpretation of archeological evidence, which includes the reluctance to distinguish projectile tools found within female burials as intended for hunting (or fighting) , this paper joins others in urging the necessity to reevaluate archeological evidence, to reassess ethnographic evidence, to question the dichotomous use of ‘hunting and gathering,’ and to deconstruct the general “man the hunter” narrative.

Of the 63 foraging societies with clear descriptions of hunting strategies, 79% of them demonstrated female hunting. The widespread presence of female hunting suggests that females play an instrumental role in hunting, further adding to the data that women contribute disproportionately to the total caloric intake of many foraging groups. Additionally, over 70% of hunting done by females is interpreted as intentional, meaning that females play an active and important role in hunting—and the teaching of hunting—even if they use different tools and employ different acquisition strategies. For example, among the Aka, women’s participation in net-hunting was required, whereas men’s participation was not.

It’s clear from these data that hunter-gatherer societies do not show a strict division of labor, though I’d like to see data on the frequency of hunts in which women participate, not just the frequency of societies in which women hunt.  Men still do most of the hunting and most of the big-game hunting, but this shows only moderate rather than extensive division of labor.

Since then, critiques of the data and conclusions of Anderson et al. have appeared first in a series of tweets by Vivek V. Venkataraman in  the original post and then in a paper in Aporia Magazine by the pseudonymous “Alexander”.

Now Venkataraman and one of his students have provided a longer critique of Anderson et al. that isn’t yet peer-reviewed, but is on Venkataraman’s blog. I can’t remember where I found this quote from him, as I copied it out a while back, but it links to the critique that is online:

A group of who know the literature have been discussing this paper on Twitter. One of us along with his students has developed a critique of many of the data problems and data censoring. For example, the paper is on hunter-gatherers but at least five groups in Table 1 are not hunter-gatherers. Here is the link: https://www.vivekvenkataraman.com/blog/2023/7/5/debunking-a-debunking

Venkataraman is an assistant professor in the Department of Anthropology and Archaeology of the University of Calgary. His university webpage describes his interests:

Dr. Venkataraman is an evolutionary anthropologist who is broadly interested in the evolution of the human diet and food systems, and their relation to life history and behavior. He is assistant director of the Guassa Gelada Research Project ,and also the co-founder and co-PI of the Orang Asli Health and Lifeways Project (OAHeLP)

As I said I’d keep people posted as analysis of the Anderson et al. paper proceeds, here’s the latest (click to read):

The general tenor of all the criticism so far is that Anderson et al. selected a group of hunter-gatherer societies that wasn’t random, and was weighted towards those in which women hunted. Further, the critics have argued that Anderson et al.’s literature analysis was slipshod, exaggerating the propensity of women who did hunt to take bigger game. This paper (and remember that it is neither submitted nor peer-reviewed) makes the same criticisms. I’ll give a few excerpts, all indented. Where bolding is mine, I say so.

We present a critique of the methodology employed by Anderson et al (2023) in their study of women’s hunting in foraging societies. When this new article was published, my students (undergraduate Jordie Hoffman and master’s student Kyle Farquharson) and I were putting the final touches on our own cross-cultural study of women’s hunting. This paper will be posted as a pre-print shortly. Given our recent immersion in this literature, we feel well-positioned to comment on this new study. We have identified a number of issues with Anderson et al’s (2023) methodology. While we applaud their investigation of this important issue, and we thank the authors for drawing our attention to sources that we ourselves had not discovered, we believe the main conclusions of their paper are invalidated by these issues.

We argue two main points about this paper:

-The sampling methodology is likely biased toward reports of female hunters, producing an inflated estimate of the frequency of women’s hunting (80%). More realistic estimates are offered.

-The claim that women commonly participate in the hunting of large game does not hold up under closer scrutiny.

This critique is by no means exhaustive. It does not deal with other significant problems about this paper, including the misrepresentation of consensus views on divisions of labor in foragers and of the intellectual history of Man the Hunter (Venkataraman 2021). Other critiques may be read here and here.

Here is their own range of estimates of hunter-gatherer societies in which women hunted:

Anderson et al (2023) argue that 50/63 (80%) forager societies show evidence of women’s hunting. For the estimate of 80% to hold up across all foragers, their sample of 63 societies would need to be representative of the broader literature. However, there are a number of societies with detailed information on subsistence behavior, including hunting, that were not included in their study. These might be found in sources with large compilations of foraging behavior such as Kaplan et al (2000) and Kraft et al (2021), among others. Dozens of such societies could appear in such a list. Because there are so many populations with ‘explicit’ – if not in-depth – descriptions of foraging returns, we are not confident that Anderson et al’s (2023) sampling strategy was not biased toward reports of women hunters. The lack of description in their methods does little to allay this concern.

ESTIMATING THE FREQUENCY OF WOMEN’S HUNTING [JAC: the bolding below is mine]

Based on our forthcoming paragraph-level survey of women’s hunting (Hoffman et al. in prep) using the Human Relations Area Files (HRAF), we found that reports of women’s hunting were genuinely rare. Our analysis revealed only 232 paragraphs across 53 societies in this large database. If we consider data from the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample (SCCS), which consists of 186 societies, we found that 29 societies had evidence of women’s hunting. This translates to a frequency of 15.5%( 29/186). We also conducted a search on the D-PLACE database, focusing on the “Sex Difference: Hunting” variable. Out of 965 societies, only 14 (1.4%) were coded for the presence of women’s hunting. Of those 14 groups, 12 were hunter-gatherers, and the other two were agriculturalists. If we treat 965 as a reasonable denominator for the number of societies and consider the 53 societies we found, this would raise the frequency of women’s hunting to 5.5% (53/965). This estimate covers all subsistence types. If we consider only the forager cases (n=391), the frequency would be calculated as 13.5% (53/391). Finally, Koster et al (2020) synthesized data on hunting across 40 small-scale societies. There was little data on hunting by women. Though the authors caution against generalizing about sex differences based on low female sample sizes, it is important to recognize that much of the data in that study came from well-studied populations (e.g. Ache, Batek, Pume). This would suggest the behavior is indeed uncommon.

Our search of reports on women’s hunting using the SCCS in the HRAF revealed nearly an 80% match with the ethnographies found by Anderson et al (2023). This convergence suggests our study and that of Anderson et al (2023) have synthesized the vast majority of ethnographic reports on women’s hunting, and do not support the idea that there is a large unsampled literature on the topic. Therefore, assuming that 80% of the 328 societies unsampled by Anderson et al (2023) will also show evidence of women’s hunting is not warranted.

In summary, the frequency of women’s hunting depends on the type of calculation performed. We argue that any reasonable calculation puts this number well below 20%. Our findings confirm previous cross-cultural work on the topic and contrast starkly with those of Anderson et al’s (2023) estimate of 80%, which, we suggest, likely results from biased sampling.

Venkataraman and Farquharson (henceforth, V&F) then show several coding errors or “problematic cases” in the data of Anderson et al. that seem to show that their original paper recorded “women hunting” when the original data didn’t show it. But V&F also note one paper in which women apparently hunted big game but it wasn’t recorded as such. In general, though, their list of eight “miscodes” tend to exaggerate the frequency of societies in which women hunt. Here are just three cases they point out:

Ganij

In Table 1, the authors reference Hewlett (1996), which has only one reference to the Ganij in a paragraph that is about breastfeeding. We were unable to locate any mention of women’s hunting.

Lardil

Memmott et al (2008) focused on fishing techniques and the trapping of fish, dugong, and turtle. The latter two prey types would, in fact, be quite large and perhaps better classified as large game. However, it was not explicitly noted that women were participating in the hunting.

Kikuyu

This Bantu ethnic group from Kenya are agriculturalists, not foragers.

If this is true, it’s embarrassing for the original authors.

Finally, the rest of the preprint is a society-by-society analysis of whether women hunted large game. The consensus of anthropologists before Anderson et al.’s paper is that this was quite rare, and V&F support that conclusion, by criticizing Anderson et al.’s analysis.  I’ll end with V&F’s conclusion (bolding is theirs):

We have critiqued a number of aspects of the Anderson et al (2023) paper. We find, on the whole, that their data do not support their conclusions. We have argued that their sampling methodology suffers from significant problems related to sampling bias. Moreover, a re-examination of their data contradict the conclusion that one-third of their sample of foraging societies show evidence of female participation in large-game hunting. With the exception of a few cases (such as the Agta and some Arctic cases, totaling roughly six), the data do not present compelling ethnographic examples of women participating habitually in large-game hunting; and in virtually every case, hunting is communal. The findings of Anderson et al (2023) do not pose a significant challenge to current consensus views on divisions of labor among foragers.

Here we see a paradigm of how science is done: the use of replication, in this case of literature analysis.  First, data are presented, then several authors go through the original data with a fine-tooth comb and conclude that it doesn’t support the original conclusions. No doubt Anderson et al. will respond to this spate of criticism.

One thing that should not be taken from this back-and-forth is that any authors have a substantial investment in finding the results they did. We have no idea of whether there was confirmation bias in any of these papers, though time will tell if there was cherry-picking of data.

Along with that goes a caveat that we shouldn’t say that “men are better” if they hunt more often, or hunt big game. The roles of both men and women even if separated by task, are both absolutely essential for these societies to function. If it turns out that men hunt mor, and take bigger game than Anderson et al concluded, that doesn’t mean that men had a more “important” role.

19 thoughts on “Still more critique of the PLOS article on women hunting in hunter-gatherer societies

  1. I appreciate these analyses – though I only commented now. I’m just waiting for someone to dispute the sexes, or suggest they were really trans – as some do in our day and age.

  2. About your last paragraph this puzzles me: folks who argue against division of labour between the sexes seem to be motivated by the apparent sexism of this division. But if men do most of the dangerous hunting, isn’t this evidence that forager societies consider men relatively expendable and women more important to the culture? I get it that other misogynist cultural practices often go along with that division of labour, but the division itself doesn’t seem sexist.

    1. Division suggests difference. Difference is not allowed. Except when difference suggests diversity. Then diversity welcomes division.

  3. I am not sure whether men are considered more “expendable” or simply that men are larger, faster, and stronger, and thus better suited to hunting (especially big game). I have no dog in this fight.

    1. I remember in reading actual scholarship on this subject, that risk avoidance was a strong consideration. It certainly make sense from a population maintenance perspective.

  4. “If it turns out that men hunt more, and take bigger game than Anderson et al concluded, that doesn’t mean that men had a more ‘important’ role.”

    Quite so, and yet this simplistic, almost childish, equation seems quite a common feature of wokery today. A couple of examples from other areas:

    1. The denigration of the Mercator world-map projection (and, almost always, mindless boosterism for the Peters projection) because Mercator exaggerates the size of all areas towards the poles, and thus relatively underestimates size of areas around the equator. There’s often a lot of nonsense about this being a deliberate ploy by “the West” to (literally) belittle tropical countries, but behind all the conspiracy theories is the unexamined assumption that *size* is the only thing that matters in a world map, and by contrast that if size is correct, then it doesn’t matter if other parameters — shape, distance, orientation — are distorted to any degree you like. Simply put, in these people’s minds it’s important that Africa looks bigger on a map, because that means it’s more important.

    2. The use in academese of capital letters in the words “Indigenous” and “Black”, while deliberately always lower-casing “white” even when used in an exactly parallel sense. To such people, there is no understanding of the orthographic principle that capitalizing a common noun or adjective is a way of distinguishing a specialized sense from a general one. There is only the childish idea that giving something a capital letter makes it more important, or shows that the writer regards the thing it designates with more respect, than if they had used lower-case.

  5. Harold Raimis’ last film”Year One” is a puerile, bawdy, but intermittently funny look at a fictional hunter/gatherer society. Jack Black plays a completely incompetent hunter who is demoted to gatherer. Then he is expelled from the tribe along with his gatherer buddy, played by Michael Cera, after which they have a series of deeply silly adventures involving Cain and Abel, Abraham, Sodom and Gomorrah, etc.. In its best line, Jack Black, making a play for a slave girl, asks her “When do you get off?”, and she replies “Never”.

  6. So this opens up some other considerations which might seem a bit sexist but it might also be true. In the modern world, most hunting is done for sport and most sports-hunting is done by men (at least that seems to be the case). Meanwhile, women tend to do home-making. Of course there are exceptions, but I am referring to what seems more common. So aren’t these a fair expression of what men simply prefer to do versus what women prefer to do? This is would be about what men and women do in their leisure time, basically, and during those times we presumably do what we like, and it seems more a stretch to say that we do these things during leisure time bc that is what society expects of us. Put together, these activities do transfer to benefits for the family and society. There is meat in the freezer, and there are vegetables in the garden, and it works out by simply letting individuals do what they like to do.
    So why not project some of that upon hunter-gatherer societies? They need meat and stone-knapped tools. They also need grains, medicinal plants, and they need clothing. So in h-g societies, that could all be allocated in large part by what individuals prefer to do. It wouldn’t be that in in a given h-g tribe the men are thinking ‘I am relatively expendable to the gene pool, so I will be the one who does dangerous stuff like hunting’. No, they would be thinking ‘I enjoy hunting, so I’m going to do that’.

    1. I’d say: if I’m a successful hunter, I’ll be a more attractive mate.

      I know that in several traditional societies (not just H/G) a male that provides meat (and chocolate added in more modern ones 🙂 ) is an attractive proposition to many a female.
      The notion that great hunters enjoy high status and have more offspring is well established, IIRC.
      (for all clarity: no, I don’t hunt)

  7. I have been following this on the WEIT site, but not reading the original studies. One aspect that is missing for me is the impact of the biome on the research. It seems possible that young fit trained women could hunt squirrels, chipmunks, rabbits, ground birds, fish wifh nets and the like. It does not seem likely that most fit women could throw a projectile with enough force from a distance to take on a bison as effectively as men. Do some biomes have a dearth of small game? Is that taken into account? What is the birth rate? Are women constantly pregnant and nursing for a couple of decades? That would prevent robust athletic activity.

    While liberal-minded contemporaries may believe separate activities based on gender are equally valuable, that is not what humans have assumed historically. While western civilization has made enormous progress for women’s rights, the default attitude is that women’s work is lower status. If someone denies that, they are simply obtuse and I will not waste effort arguing with them. It does not follow though, that I want to see scientists looking for equal contributions in hunting or any other “male” activities that don’t actually exist. That is no way to further women’s or human rights. It actually reinforces that hunting is a more elite activity than gathering to insist that it is preferable that women engaged in hunting- if it was not so.

    1. Evidence is that bison-hunting was a collaboration. Women, as indispensably as men, would cooperatively drive bison off cliffs where the manly men would dispatch the injured immobilized animals with wooden spears and clubs. There is no evidence from research at Head-Smashed-In Buffalo Jump in Alberta that men could kill bison at a distance in classical hunter fashion with the short simple bows used on the Plains. This makes me doubt that men actually did much he-man hunting of large game until firearms were adopted.

      1. For the majority of the period under discussion, bows had not yet come into general use. So the plains horseback buffalo hunt was obviously not happening, with they key elements not yet introduced.
        On the other hand, there is plenty of evidence of paleolithic hunting in North America with bone and fluted stone points, including mastodons. The sites do not suggest that driving the animals off of cliffs was the most common method. I suspect hunting a mastodon involved harassing and tiring it out as a pack of wolves would do, but some guy still needs to get close enough to deliver a fatal blow with a spear. That is a fairly hard core activity from any perspective.

        Also, Paleoindians spent a lot of time making and perfecting large spear points. Nobody needs a six inch long spear point to hunt marmots.

        1. Some years ago, I saw a YouTube video of a group of Africans taking down a bull elephant with nothing but spears. They surrounded the animal and threw spears into it from all directions until it finally collapsed from exhaustion and blood loss. Then they moved in for the kill. I imagine that is how mammoths and other large animals were hunted. I always thought the “driving them over cliffs” theory was a bit far-fetched. I don’t know if the video is still available as seeing it once was enough for me.

          1. There are definitely places where evidence of driving herds of animals off of cliffs can be found. No doubt doing so requires specific geography, timing, and species of animals susceptible to the tactic.

    2. Agree totally. I also get tired of all talk of “men do all the dangerous stuff”, like women are wrapped in cotton wool or something. There is nothing “safe” about giving birth to baby after baby after baby in a society with no modern medicine, antibiotics and blood-clotting drugs! Men always conveniently forget the high death-in-childbirth rate pre modern times.

      1. I think everybody’s got it backwards. I think it’s entirely possible that there have been many societies where women did the dangerous child bearing and the dangerous hunting. It’s just that they generally die out quickly because all the women are dead.

  8. “This Bantu ethnic group from Kenya are agriculturalists, not foragers.”
    The very reason for the ‘Bantu Expansion’, agriculturalists simply outbreed hunter/gatherers/foragers, for obvious reasons.

  9. I don’t understand why everybody is so invested in this. Yes, it’s anthropologically of interest whether women did or didn’t hunt in olden days, but I can’t see any reason why it is of relevance to modern society. If every single hunter gatherer society ever followed the stereotype, would it make any difference at all to the argument that women deserve all the same opportunities as men, that they deserve equal pay and that everybody in society should be treated fairly regardless of sex?

  10. I suspect this whole issue is a straw man. The archetype of a female hunter goes back thousands of years, to the Greek god Artemis if not earlier. Why anyone would be surprised to find that women participated in hunting is beyond me.

    In ancient art Artemis was usually depicted as a girl or young maiden with a hunting bow and quiver of arrows.

    https://www.theoi.com/Olympios/Artemis.html

Leave a Comment

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *